Dakota Reis, B.A.

Project Coordinator

The Cognition, Affect, and Temperament Lab

Pennsylvania State University

Photo by Mark Melendez

About

I am currently a project coordinator working for Dr. Koraly Perez-Edgar in the Cognition, Affect, and Temperament Lab at Pennsylvania State University. In 2019, I completed my Bachelor of Arts with summa cum laude at Rutgers University - Newark, where I worked as a research assistant for Dr. Vanessa LoBue and Dr. Lauren Leotti in the Child Study Center.Much of my research focuses on how parent-child interactions influence children's motivation and persistence. Particularly, how parent's use of language and scaffolding directs their child's allocation of effort. Young children spend a considerable amount of time with their parents within the first few years of life. However, as they get older and spend more time away from home (i.e., school, sports, hobbies), other adults begin to steer children's motivational frameworks. I would love to direct my future research towards exploring how these interactions with other adult's influence children's allocation of effort across various contexts.My training includes techniques such as Behavioral Coding, Eye-tracking, fNIRS, ECG, and EEG.

Personal Interests

Outside of research, I'm also an avid rock-climber with over 7-years of climbing experience and 3 years of coaching experience. I started my climbing career at a small gym located in New Jersey, Garden State Rocks, and I immediately fell in love with the sport. Soon after, I got a job at that very same gym teaching youth classes and the rest was history. By the end of my career, I managed to coach our small youth team to the USAC Youth Nationals 2-years in a row, beating some of the largest teams in the U.S.Throughout my time spent in the climbing industry, no other job has provided me with greater satisfaction. I find it deeply rewarding to see the kids I've worked with grow into young adults that continue to challenge themselves, train hard, and maintain a healthy relationship with the sport.While I no longer coach full-time, my love for the sport still remains and I would love to direct future research towards developing training efficiencies for climbing athletes.

To learn more about my climbing journey, please click here

Research Projects

PCAT Pilot Study

PCAT Longitudinal Study

Natural Variability in Parent-Child Puzzle Play



PCAT: Parent-to-Child Anxiety Transmission

Active Data Collection: October 2022

NIH R56MH126349

Anxiety is a developmental disorder grounded in and facilitated by our parents attentional biases towards risk. While other factors (i.e., fetal programming and genetics) also contribute to the early emergence of anxiety, current interventions cannot directly target these risk factors. However, a wide body of literature has identified specific parenting behaviors linked to the emergence of childhood anxiety. These dynamic, social, and adaptive dyadic interactions may act as a behavioral conduit for parental expressions of risk biases.This study, in collaboration with Dr. Susan Perlman and her team at Washington University, St. Louis, is designed to focus on two instances of social dynamics as mechanisms for anxiety transmission: (1) Dyadic Synchrony and (2) Emotional Modeling


Our Main Questions

1) How do patterns of dyadic social dynamics vary across parent-child pairs?

2) To what extent does variation in dyadic patterns help predict anxiety risk?

3) Over time, can we predict socioemotional profiles and anxiety risk from earlier patterns of dynamic dyadic interactions?


Study Description

Participation in this study requires three steps; (1) a remote interview with the primary parent, (2) an in-lab visit scheduled with the primary parent and child, and (3) an additional set of questionnaires given to the secondary parent. The in-lab session takes about 2-3 hours to complete. During which, parents and their children (ages 4-7) are asked to play several games while we collect behavioral, fNIRS, and Mobile Eye-tracking data.

Tasks & Measures


TaskParticipantMeasures
KBIT2ChildIntelligence
DB-DOSChild, ParentBehavioral (emotion and distress), Neural (fNIRS)
TSSTChild, ParentBehavioral (emotional and distress), Attentional (MET)
FlankerChildBehavioral (executive functioning), Neural (fNIRS)
JumbleChildBehavioral, Attentional (MET)
PosnerChildBehavioral
QuestionnaireParent**MINI, ***FIGS, *HBQ, *CBQ, *BIQ, *PRQ, *PDHS-R, *ACEs, **STAI, **BAI, **COVID-19 Impact Survey, **PSS, **CCNES, ****FACES, ****CHAOS

*About Child, **About Primary Parent, ***About Secondary Parent, ****About Family



PCAT: Parent-to-Child Anxiety Transmission

Pending Data Collection: October 2022

NIH R01MH130007

Anxiety is a developmental disorder grounded in and facilitated by our parents attentional biases towards risk. While other factors (i.e., fetal programming and genetics) also contribute to the early emergence of anxiety, current interventions cannot directly target these risk factors. However, a wide body of literature has identified specific parenting behaviors linked to the emergence of childhood anxiety. These dynamic, social, and adaptive dyadic interactions may act as a behavioral conduit for parental expressions of risk biases.This study, in collaboration with Dr. Susan Perlman and her team at Washington University, St. Louis, is designed to focus on two instances of social dynamics as mechanisms for anxiety transmission: (1) Dyadic Synchrony and (2) Emotional Modeling

Tasks & Measures


TaskParticipantMeasures
KBIT2ChildIntelligence
DB-DOSChild, ParentBehavioral (emotion and distress), Neural (fNIRS), Psychophysiological (RSA)
TSSTChild, ParentBehavioral (emotional and distress), Attentional (MET), Psychophysiological (RSA)
FlankerChildBehavioral (executive functioning), Neural (EEG)
JumbleChildBehavioral, Attentional (MET)
QuestionnairesParent**MINI, ***FIGS, *HBQ, *CBQ, *BIQ, *PRQ, *PDHS-R, *ACEs, **STAI, **BAI, **COVID-19 Impact Survey, **PSS, **CCNES, ****FACES, ****CHAOS

*About Child, **About Primary Parent, ***About Secondary Parent, ****About Family

Natural Variability in Parent-Child Puzzle Play

DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.733895

Introduction

Spatial skills are central for everyday functioning, allowing us to encode the features, locations, and orientations of objects, as well as mentally manipulate this information. Spatial skills not only make it possible to interpret maps and diagrams, but also they are important predictors of later achievement across diverse STEM disciplines. For decades, research has documented a significant and robust relationship between spatial skills and mathematics performance over the course of development. As a result, identifying factors that might influence the development of spatial skills in early childhood has received a great deal of attention.

Since the COVID-19 pandemic put a number of constraints on our ability to collect data with children in the lab, new approaches to study child development were necessary. Here, we used a videoconference platform (Zoom) to study spatial play at home, along with the spatial and constructive toys that parents typically choose for their children. The existing studies that have examined children and their parents playing with toys in the home have focused on the relationship between the frequency of spatial play and parent support, or parent language and children’s performance on spatial tasks. This study sought to characterize the variability in various factors linked to spatial skills in children during their naturalistic play with spatial toys they had at home.We explored variability in the types of puzzles families of 3 and 4-year-old children (N=30) interact with in their homes, and the nature of those parent-child interactions during naturalistic play. The study was conducted over Zoom, and we simply recorded parents and their children as they played.


Our Main Question

How does variability in spatial toys found at home influence parental behavior and language during puzzle play?


Coding

Coders watched the recorded play session to categorize the puzzles’ difficulty and to identify instances of specific child and parent behaviors. Children’s insertion attempts, parental scaffolding behavior, and parental language were all coded using Datavyu.


Puzzle Difficulty

There were five nested dimensions, each that were assigned a value of 0 (easiest) to 1 (most difficult). The first dimension was Puzzle type, which referred to whether the puzzle was a board puzzle or jigsaw puzzle. Puzzles were further coded for whether or not they had a tray. Puzzles that had a tray were then coded for whether they contained a background image that matched the puzzle piece or no background image. Puzzles that contained large pieces (i.e., pieces that were larger than the child’s hands) were considered easier than standard jigsaw puzzles. Finally, puzzles were coded for whether or not they involved interlocking pieces. These dimensions were then summed.The number of pieces in each puzzle was also coded from the videos of the play session and from the puzzle photos submitted through the Qualtrics questionnaire. If information about the number of pieces was missing, an online search was conducted to identify the puzzle and obtain the specifications from the manufacturer’s Web site.A puzzle difficulty composite score then was created by adding the binary values of all the coded difficulty dimensions and a code ranging from 1 to 5 based on the number of pieces the puzzles contained. The final puzzle difficulty score ranged from 1 to 10, where a score of 10 was the most difficult.

Parent Behaviors

Parent scaffolding events were identified throughout the play session. Scaffolding events consisted of the sum of four different behaviors: (1) removing a piece that was placed in an incorrect space by the child, (2) helping by handing the child individual pieces or rotating pieces for the child, (3) pointing or outlining to a piece or a space in the puzzle, (4) pointing or outlining to the pictorial representation of the puzzle. We created a total scaffolding score by summing the instances of each of these behaviors. In addition to scaffolding, we also coded instances where parents inserted a piece into the puzzle for the child. This final code was not included in the total scaffolding behavior score.


Parental Language

Spatial language included any mention of spatial dimensions, shapes, locations and directions, orientations and transformations, spatial features, and properties. Examples of utterances coded as containing spatial language are “where’s the flat edge?”, “but I think you might need to rotate it a little,” and “this is a big puzzle." We only included spatial terms that were in reference to the construction of the puzzles and omitted terms that were unrelated to the puzzle.In addition to spatial language, we also coded praise and persistence-focused language, which have been linked to more general engagement and persistence in children. Praise included utterances that positively evaluated the child or the child’s actions (e.g., “You’re good at puzzles”; “good job”), or utterances that expressed general positive valence toward the child but not directed at any specific action (e.g., “Awesome!”; “Yay!”). Persistence-focused language consisted of utterances that were focused on trying or repeated attempts to complete a goal-directed action. Frequently, this consisted of phrases that explicitly referred to acts of trying (e.g., “You’re trying so hard!”).


Child Behaviors

Children's insertion attempts were identified throughout the play session. An insertion attempt was defined as the first time the child took one puzzle piece and proceeded to either join it with one or more additional pieces or place it in an opening in a puzzle tray. An insertion attempt could be either successful if the child placed the piece in the correct space or unsuccessful if the child failed to insert the piece correctly and proceeded to place the piece back down on the floor or table. Each time the child attempted to insert the same piece in any opening or location was counted as a single event, which ended when the child either successfully inserted the piece or placed it down.After coding initial insertion attempts, a trained coder went back to each insertion attempt and counted the number of times the children unsuccessfully attempted to insert a single piece before either successfully inserting it or putting it down. An unsuccessful attempt was coded every time the child tried to insert the piece into a different place in the puzzle or in the same place but in a different orientation. A different orientation was defined as a rotation of the piece more than 90 degrees.


Conclusions

The most noteworthy finding from this descriptive study is the enormous variability we observed in both children and parents’ behaviors, and in the puzzles they selected for play. This study is the first of its kind in provide detailed characterization of the kinds of puzzles children have at their homes as well as the variability in parents’ and children’s behavior while engaging in home puzzle play. However, despite the large amount of variability reported here, there are some relationships documented in previous literature that were also evident in the current sample, speaking to their robustness.For example, similar to our results, several studies have shown that parents provide more assistance to younger versus older children during puzzle-building tasks, suggesting that parents might adjust their behavior to fit different children’s needs. Finally, we found several gender differences suggesting that girls were more persistent than boys, making more attempts to place pieces into the puzzle after failure, and that parents used more persistence-focused language with girls than with boys and gave girls more difficult puzzles. Gender differences in children’s spatial ability and spatial play have also been reported in previous literature, usually attributing more advanced spatial skills to boys than girls, but these findings are controversial and require further research.

GET IN TOUCH

I would love to hear from you, so please send me an email if you would like to collaborate or ask a question!

Photo by Courtney R. Billig

Curriculum Vitae

For the full PDF version, please click hereLast Updated: October 2022


Education

2019 - 2021



2015 - 2018

B.A. in Psychology

GPA: 3.93, summa cum laude


A.A. in Social and Health Science

Rutgers University, Newark



Brookdale Community College

Professional Experience

2021 - Present



2018 - 2021



2019 - 2021



2020



2016-2019

Cognition, Affect, and Temperament Lab

Project Coordinator


HMH JFK Medical Center

Registrar


Child Study Center Laboratory

Research Assistant, Undergraduate


Child Study Center Laboratory

Summer Internship in Developmental Science


Garden State Rocks

Routesetting Manager & Head Coach

Pennsylvania State University



Edison, NJ



Rutgers University, Newark



Rutgers University, Newark



Morganville, NJ


Publications and Posters

2022

Anderson, A.J., Furtado, E., Schneider, C., Iroh, U., Thiem, S., Reis, D., Edgar-Perez, K., Perlman, S.B. (2022, November). Now and then: The role of neural and cognitive efficiency during behavioral control and in socially inhibited children. Short talk presented at the 2022 meeting of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, San Diego, CA.


2021

Leotti, L., Pochinki, N., Reis, D., Bonawitz, E., & LoBue, V. (2021). Learning about germs in a global pandemic: Children’s knowledge and avoidance of contagious illness before and after COVID-19. Cognitive Development, 59, 101090. Publisher's Version.


Pochinki, N., Agbamu, S., Reis, D., Avalos, R., Starling, S., B Velarde, Yassein, F., Oakes, L., Casola, M., LoBue, V. 2021. Can Parental Scaffolding During Spatial Play Predict Children’s Spatial Skills? Poster presented at the Biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Virtual (due to COVID-19).


Pochinki N, Reis D, Casasola M, Oakes LM, LoBue V. Natural Variability in Parent-Child Puzzle Play at Home. Front Psychol. 2021 Sep 16;12:733895. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.733895. PMID: 34603155; PMCID: PMC8483633.


Reis, D., Leotti, L., Pochinki, N., Bonawitz, E., & LoBue, V. (2021, April). Learning about Germs in a Global Pandemic: Children’s Knowledge and Behavior Before and After COVID-19. Poster presented at the Biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Virtual (due to COVID-19).

Awards and Honors

2019 - 2021


2020 - 2021


2020


2020

Rutger's University Dean's List


Psi-Chi Honor's Society


Summer Internship in Developmental Science


Child Study Center Laboratory

Teaching



Last Updated: October 2022


Philosophy

When I was in high school, I graduated with a 2.9 GPA. Most of my teachers were understandably frustrated with my performance. I participated in class, was attentive, and took detailed notes, but I never did my homework and I rarely studied. My teachers would all agree I had potential, but I "just didn't apply myself." Despite this, none of them bothered to ask why or tried to understand what could be hindering my performance.That is why I believe the key to a successful learning environment is actively fostering a supportive environment. To formulate effective strategies for learning, you need to understand who your students are as individuals. You need to connect with them, learn what motivates them, what demotivates them, and their struggles. Otherwise, you miss those student's that might have had potential, but were more worried about stressors outside the classroom.

Experience

Pennsylvania State University

State College, PA

2022 fNIRS Training for PCAT
2022 Mobile Eye-tracking Training for PCAT


Rutgers University

Newark, NJ

2020 Teaching Assistant: 102 - Principles of Psychology 102
2020 Learning Assistant: 302 - Quantitative Methods in Psychology


Garden State Rocks

Morganville, NJ

2019 Head Coach: Youth-Climbing Team (ages 8-18)
2016 Assistant Coach: Youth-Climbing Team (ages 8-18)

Climbing

My Origin Story

Not yet written

Photo by Mark Melendez

Tick List

New York

Ice Pond

Badger (V5)
Cowabunga (V8)
Donatello Right (V5)
Flip Phone (V8)
Intergalactic (V8)
Mongoose (V6)
Raphael Left (V7)
Raphael Right (V4)
Spatula O’Keefe (V9)
Stool Sample (V6)
The Churro (V8)
The Burro (V7)

Pennsylvania

Bodine Mountain

Badger (V5)
Cowabunga (V8)
Donatello Right (V5)
Flip Phone (V8)
Intergalactic (V8)
Mongoose (V6)
Raphael Left (V7)
Raphael Right (V4)
Spatula O’Keefe (V9)
Stool Sample (V6)
The Churro (V8)
The Burro (V7)

Coal Run

Second Amendment (V3)
Sinful Nature (V7)

Falls Creek

Falling Skies (V9-)
Jilted (V9)
One Pump Chump (V8)
Parallel Lives (V7)

Hunter's Rock

Agape (V5)
Alcoholic Recovery (V6)
Curse a Real Sword (V7)
Hunger Artist, The (V9)
Levitation Complication (V10)
Looks Like a Turtle (V5)
Mushroom Tattoo (V6)
Standard American Accent (V7)
The First Church of Appliantology (V7-)
Tri Angle Roof (V5)

Long Valley

Breathe Underwater (V5)
High Priestesses (V8)
Oofta (V6)
Sleeping with Ghosts (V9)
Wages of War (V6)

Rock Run

Bad Voodoo (V5)
Elephants of Hunger (V9)
Gemini Egg (V6)
Glass Slipper (V5)
Human Pony Girl (V7)
The Horn (V5)
The Wave (V4)
Wailing Wall (V9)

Sunfish Pond

12 Rounds (V10)
8 Minute Abs (V8)
Archaic Torso of Apollo (V6)
Chinese Water Torture (V9-)
D.A.B (V8)
Leap of Faith (V7)
Leroy (The King) (V8)
Libations (V7)
Secret Splendor (V4)
Soul Child (V10)
Tailspin (V9-)
Trials of The Past (V7)
Woman King (V3)